Skip to main content
Log in

Syntax and Morphology in Danish-Speaking Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The current study examined delays in syntax and morphology, and vocabulary, in autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Children ages 4–6 years with ASD (n = 21) and typical development (n = 21), matched on nonverbal mental age, completed five language tasks. The ASD group had significant delays in both syntax and morphology, and vocabulary measures, with significant within-group heterogeneity; furthermore, syntactic and morphological measures were impaired even for subgroups matched on vocabulary. Children in the ASD group without early language delay showed syntactic and morphological impairment, with intact performance on vocabulary and sentence repetition. Findings indicate that syntactic and morphological impairments are a significant concern for high-functioning children with ASD, and may be overlooked if language evaluation focuses exclusively on vocabulary.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The main difference between ICD-10 criteria for Childhood Autism and DSM-5 criteria for Autism Spectrum Disorder is that the two first subcriteria of ICD-10, (a) deficits within social interaction, and (b) deficits within language and communication, have been collapsed into the first subcriterion of DSM-5: deficits within social-communication and social interaction. All children diagnosed with ICD-10 Childhood Autism in this study would qualify for a diagnosis of DSM-5 Autism Spectrum Disorder.

  2. The similarities between Danish and English grammar made a relatively direct translation viable. The main morphological differences are that Danish does not have present progressive and third person marking, but does mark nouns for gender. As such, items testing present progressive and third person marking were removed. The publisher prohibited the inclusion of any additional items, so the CELF did not include items measuring gender marking on nouns; this marking is acquired very early. An additional difference is that Danish has no rules for copula contraction; thus, items testing this structure were also removed. The translated version thus measures nearly all the relevant Danish morphemes. As for syntactic differences between Danish and English, in some Danish sentences (e.g., those introduced by an adverbial phrase), the verb precedes the subject. In the subtest Sentence Structure, there were no cases of this “inverted” word order, and thus all translations were straightforward. Only one item was removed from Sentence Structure, due to semantic differences. Altogether, the translated version was thought to be useful for measuring morphosyntactic knowledge of Danish.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders IV-TR (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bartolucci, G., Pierce, S. J., & Streiner, D. L. (1980). Cross-sectional studies of grammatical morphemes in autistic and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 10(1), 39–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boucher, J. (2012). Research review: Structural language in autistic spectrum disorder—characteristics and causes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 53(3), 219–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cantwell, D. P., Baker, L., Rutter, M., & Mahwood, L. (1989). Infantile autism and developmental receptive dysphasia: A comparative follow-up into middle childhood. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 19–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C. & Baird, G. (2003). Measuring early language development in pre-school children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language, 30, 213–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chouinard, B., & Cummine, J. (2016). All the world’s a stage: Evaluation of two stages of metaphor comprehension in people with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 23, 107–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clay, M. M. (1971). Sentence repetition: Elicited imitation of a controlled set of syntactic structures by four language groups. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 36(3), 85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(6), 741–748.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Giacomo, A., & Fombonne, E. (1998). Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 7(3), 131–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test, (4th ed.). Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn L., Dunn M., Styles, B., & Sewell, J. (2009). British picture vocabulary scale (BPVS-III) (3rd ed.). London: GL Assessment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eigsti, I.-M., & Bennetto, L. (2009). Grammaticality judgments in autism: Deviance or delay. Journal of Child Language, 36(5), 999–1021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eigsti, I.-M., Bennetto, L., & Dadlani, M. B. (2007). Beyond pragmatics: Morphosyntactic development in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1007–1023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 1–173. Discussion 74–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fortunato-Tavares, T., Andrade, C, R. F., Befi-Lopes, D., Limongi, S, O., Fernandes, F. D. M., & Schwartz, R. G. (2015). Syntactic comprehension and working memory in children with specific language impairment, autism or down syndrome. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 29(7), 499–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerenser, J. (2009). Language disorders in children with autism. In R. G. Schwartz (Ed.), Handbook of child language disorders (pp. 67–89). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A., Geye, H. M., & Weismer, S. E. (2005). The role of language and communication impairments within autism. In P. Fletcher & J. F. Miller (Eds.), Developmental theory and language disorders (pp. 73–93). Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Grønborg, A., Køhler, I., Lund, J., & Møller, B. H. (1987). Dansk Impressiv Morfologisk test (DIM). Herning, DK: Special-pædagogisk forlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, E. J., & Andersen, B. H. (2009). Et sociologisk værktøj : introduktion til den kvantitative metode (2nd ed.). Kbh.: Hans Reitzel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harper-Hill, K., Copland, D., & Arnott, W. (2013). Do spoken nonword and sentence repetition tasks discriminate language impairment in children with an ASD? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7, 265–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B.M., & Risley, T.R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publ. Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E., & Tian, C. (2005). Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. Journal of Communication Disorders, 38, 271–278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howlin, P. (1984). The acquisition of grammatical morphemes in autistic children: A critique and replication of the findings of Bartolucci, Pierce, and Streiner, 1980. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14(2), 127–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howlin, P. (2005). Outcomes in Autism Spectrum Disorders. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.). Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders: Diagnosis, development, neurobiology, and behavior (3rd ed., Vol. 1), (pp. 201–220). Hoboken, NJ, US: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Russell, J. (1997). Language profiles in children with autism. Autism: The international Journal of Research and Practice, 1(1), 57–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kidd, E., Brandt, S., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Object relatives made easy: A cross-linguistic comparison of the constraints influencing young children’s processing of relative clauses. Language and Cogntive Processes, 22(6), 860–897.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, S. H., Paul, R., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Lord, C. (2014). Language and communication in autism. In F. Volkmar, R. Paul & A. Klin (Eds.), Handbook on autism and pervasive developmental disorders (3rd ed., pp. 230–262). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An Investigation of Language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitve Processes, 16(2–3), 287–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L. B. (2007). Processing limitations and the grammatical profile of children with specific language impairment. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 35, 139–171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linebarger, M. C., Schwartz, M. F., & Saffran, E. M. (1983). Sensitivity to grammatical structure in so-called agrammatic aphasics. Cognition, 13(3), 361–392.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., … Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview—revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayo, J., Chlebowski, C., Fein, D. A., & Eigsti, I.-M. (2013). Age of first words predicts cognitive ability and adaptive skills in children with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(2), 253–264.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McGregor, K. K., Berns, A. J., Owen, A. J., Michels, S. A., Duff, D., Bahnsen, A. J., & Lloyd, M. (2012). Associations between syntax and the lexicon among children with or without ASD and language impairment. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(1), 35.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mervis, C., & Klein-Tasman, B. (2004). Methodological issues in group-matching designs: Alpha levels for control variable comparisons and measurement characteristics of control and target variables. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(1), 7–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L. (2004). Matching strategies in cognitive research with individuals with high-functioning autism: Current practices, instrument biases, and recommendations. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(1), 19–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Park, C. J., Yelland, G. W., Taffe, J. R., & Gray, K. M. (2012). Morphological and syntactic skills in language samples of preschool aged children with autism: Atypical development? International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14(2), 95–108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perovic, A., Modyanova, K., & Wexler, K. (2013). Comprehension of reflexive and personal pronouns in children with autism: A syntactic or pragmatic deficit? Applied Psycholinguistics, 34(4), 813–835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prizant, B. M. (1983). Language acquisition and communicative behavior in autism: Toward an understanding of the “whole” of it. Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 48(3), 296–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. (2008). Raven’s—educational: Coloured progressive matrices. London: Pearson Assessment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riches, N. G., Loucas, T., Baird, G., Charman, T., & Simonoff, E. (2010). Research report: Sentence repetition in adolescents with specific language impairments and autism: An investigation of complex syntax. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 45(1), 47–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social communication questionnaire (SCQ). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (1970). Autistic children: Infancy to adulthood. Seminars in Psychiatry, 2(4), 435–450.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stokes, S. F., Wong, A. M. Y., Fletcher, P., & Leonard, L. B. (2006). Nonword repetition and sentence repetition as clinical markers of specific language impairment: The case of Cantonese. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(2), 219–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (1981). Sentence comprehension in autistic children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 2(1), 5–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (2015). Defining language impairments in a subgroup of children with autism spectrum disorder. Science China Life Sciences, 58(10), 1044–1052.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1995). Attributing mental states to story characters: A comparison of narratives produced by autistic and mentally retarded individuals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16(3), 241–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomblin, B. (2011). Co-morbidity of autism and SLI: Kinds, kin and complexity. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 46(2), 127–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, L. Y., & Beisler, J. M. (1984). Research in infantile autism: A methodological problem in using language comprehension as the basis for selecting matched controls. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 23(6), 700–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiig, E. H., Secord, W. A., & Semel, E. (2004). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals, preschool (2nd ed.) San Antonio, TX: Pearson Education, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • World Health Organization. (1992). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems, 10th revision (ICD-10). Geneva: WHO.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the participating children, parents, daycare centers, and the student assistants who helped in the study.

Funding

This research was funded by Aarhus University and supported in part by two grants from Augustinus Fonden (09-2372; 10-1366) and one from Fru C. Hermansens Mindelegat (00962-0001) to the first author, and by the Danish National Research Foundation (DNRF93) to the last author. The LEGO Foundation also provided funding.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cecilia Brynskov.

Ethics declarations

Author Contributions

Study conception and design: Brynskov, Eigsti, Krøjgaard, Bohn; Acquisition of data: Brynskov, Jorgensen, Lemcke; Analysis and interpretation of data: Brynskov, Eigsti; Drafting of manuscript: Brynskov; Critical revision: Brynskov, Eigsti.

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Brynskov, C., Eigsti, IM., Jørgensen, M. et al. Syntax and Morphology in Danish-Speaking Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 47, 373–383 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2962-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2962-7

Keywords

Navigation